Study: Climate Change Skeptics Know More About Science Than Believers
M2RB:
I know you're fed up
Like a lead up for us
All they talk about is
What is going down?
What's been messed up for us?
What I look around I see blue skies
I see butterflies for us
Listen to the sound and lose it
Its sweet music and dance with me
There is beauty in the world
So much beauty in the world
Always beauty in the world
So much beauty in the world
Shake your booty boys and girls for the beauty in the world
Pick your diamond, pick your pearl
There is beauty in the world
Altogether now
We need more lovin'
We need more money, they say
Change is gonna come
Like the weather
They say
When they're in between
Notice the blue skies
Notice the butterflies
Notice me
The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks
By Dan M Kahan, Ellen Peters, Maggie Wittlin, Paul Slovic, Lisa Larrimore Ouellette, Donald Braman, and Gregory Mandel
Seeming public
apathy over climate change is often attributed to a deficit in
comprehension. The public knows too little science, it is claimed, to
understand the evidence or avoid being misled1.
Widespread limits on technical reasoning aggravate the problem by
forcing citizens to use unreliable cognitive heuristics to assess risk2.
We conducted a study to test this account and found no support for it.
Members of the public with the highest degrees of science literacy and
technical reasoning capacity were not the most concerned about climate
change. Rather, they were the ones among whom cultural polarization was
greatest. This result suggests that public divisions over climate change
stem not from the public’s incomprehension of science but from a
distinctive conflict of interest: between the personal interest
individuals have in forming beliefs in line with those held by others
with whom they share close ties and the collective one they all share in
making use of the best available science to promote common welfare.
The study collected data on the climate-change risk perceptions of a large representative sample of US adults (N=1,540).
Measures were selected to permit assessment of two competing accounts
of public opinion on climate change. One, already adverted to, can be
called the science comprehension thesis (SCT). As members of the public
do not know what scientists know, or think the way scientists think,
they predictably fail to take climate change as seriously as scientists
believe they should3.
The
alternative explanation can be referred to as the cultural cognition
thesis (CCT). CCT posits that individuals, as a result of a complex of
psychological mechanisms, tend to form perceptions of societal risks
that cohere with values characteristic of groups with which they
identify4, 5.
Whereas SCT emphasizes a conflict between scientists and the public,
CCT stresses one between different segments of the public, whose members
are motivated to fit their interpretations of scientific evidence to
their competing cultural philosophies6.
Explanations
for the public’s perceptions of climate change risk can be tested by
observational study insofar as such hypotheses imply correlations
between concern over climate change and specified individual
characteristics 7.
We instructed subjects to rate the seriousness of climate change risk
on a scale of 0 (no risk) to 10 (extreme risk), a general risk-concern
measure that furnishes a parsimonious focus for such testing8, 9.
SCT
asserts, first, that ordinary members of the public underestimate the
seriousness of climate change because of the difficulty of the
scientific evidence3.
If this is correct, concern over climate change should be positively
correlated with science literacy—that is, concern should increase as
people become more science literate.
Second, and even more
important, SCT attributes low concern with climate change to limits on
the ability of ordinary members of the public to engage in technical
reasoning. Recent research in psychology posits two discrete forms of
information processing: system 1, which involves rapid visceral
judgments that manifest themselves in various decision-making
heuristics; and system 2, which requires conscious reflection and
calculation10.
Most members of the public, according to this research, typically
employ system 1 reasoning without resorting to more effortful system 2
processing. Although system 1 works well for most daily contingencies,
ordinary citizens’ predominant reliance on heuristic rather than
analytic modes of reasoning is viewed as leading them to underestimate
climate change risks, which are remote and abstract compared with a host
of more emotionally charged risks (for example, terrorism) that the
public is thought to overestimate2, 3.
If
this position is correct, one would also expect concern with climate
change to be positively correlated with numeracy. Numeracy refers to the
capacity of individuals to comprehend and make use of quantitative
information11.
More numerate people are more disposed to use accuracy-enhancing system
2 forms of reasoning and are less vulnerable to system 1 cognitive
errors11, 12. Hence, they should, on this view, form perceptions of climate-change risk less biased towards underestimation.
These predictions were unsupported (Fig. 1). As respondents’ science-literacy scores increased, concern with climate change decreased (r=−0.05, P=0.05). There was also a negative correlation between numeracy and climate change risk (r=−0.09, P<0.01). The differences were small, but nevertheless inconsistent with SCT, which predicts effects with the opposite signs.
Figure 1: SCT prediction versus actual impact of science literacy and numeracy on climate change risk perceptions.
Contrary to SCT predictions, higher
degrees of science literacy and numeracy are associated with a small
decrease in the perceived seriousness of climate change risks. Derived
from Supplementary Table S4, Model 1. Low and high reflect values set at −1 s.d. and +1 s.d. on the composite Science literacy/numeracy scale (see Supplementary Information). Responses on the 0–10 risk scale (M=5.7, s.d.=3.4) were converted to z-scores to promote ease of interpretation. Confidence intervals reflect the 0.95 level of confidence.
CCT also generates a testable prediction. CCT posits that people who
subscribe to a hierarchical, individualistic world-view—one that ties
authority to conspicuous social rankings and eschews collective
interference with the decisions of individuals possessing such
authority—tend to be sceptical of environmental risks. Such people
intuitively perceive that widespread acceptance of such risks would
license restrictions on commerce and industry, forms of behaviour that
hierarchical individualists value. In contrast, people who hold an
egalitarian, communitarian world-view—one favouring less regimented
forms of social organization and greater collective attention to
individual needs—tend to be morally suspicious of commerce and industry,
to which they attribute social inequity. They therefore find it
congenial to believe those forms of behaviour are dangerous and worthy
of restriction4.
On this view, one would expect egalitarian communitarians to be more
concerned than hierarchical individualists with climate change risks.
Our data, consistent with previous studies6,
supported this prediction. Hierarchical individualists (subjects who
scored in the top half on both the Hierarchy and Individualism
cultural-world-view scales) rated climate change risks significantly
lower (M=3.15, s.e.m.=0.17) than did egalitarian communitarians (subjects whose scores placed them in the bottom half; M=7.4,
s.e.m.=0.13). Even controlling for scientific literacy and numeracy (as
reflected in the composite scale Science literacy/numeracy; see Supplementary Information), both Hierarchy (b=−0.46, P<0.01) and Individualism (b=−0.30, P<0.01) predicted less concern over climate change (Supplementary Table S4).
These findings were consistent, too, with previous ones showing that climate change has become highly politicized13, 14.
Cultural-world-view and political-orientation measures are modestly
correlated. Nevertheless, the impact that cultural world-views has on
climate change risk perceptions cannot be reduced to partisanship. The
mean hierarchical individualist in our sample was an Independent who
leans Republican and is slightly conservative; the mean egalitarian
communitarian was also an Independent, but one who leans Democrat and is
slightly liberal (Supplementary Fig. S4).
The difference between their respective perceptions of climate change
risk, however, significantly exceeded what political-orientation
measures alone would predict for individuals who identify themselves as
conservative Republicans and liberal Democrats (Supplementary Fig. S5).
The
finding that cultural world-views explain more variance than science
literacy and numeracy, however, does not by itself demonstrate that SCT
is less supportable than CCT. SCT asserts not merely that members of the
public lack scientific knowledge but also that they lack the habits of
mind needed to assimilate it, and are thus constrained to rely on
fallible heuristic alternatives. Proponents of this bounded-rationality
position treat cultural cognition—the conforming of beliefs to the ones
that predominate within one’s group—as simply one of the unreliable
system 1 heuristics used to compensate for the inability to assess
scientific information in a dispassionate, analytical manner15.
This
claim generates another testable prediction. If cultural cognition is
merely a heuristic substitute for scientific knowledge and system 2
reasoning, reliance on it should be lowest among those individuals whose
scientific knowledge and system 2 reasoning capacity are highest. SCT
thus implies that as science literacy and numeracy increase, the
scepticism over climate change associated with a hierarchical
individualistic world-view should lessen and the gap between people with
hierarchical individualistic world-views and those with egalitarian
communitarian ones should diminish.
However, this SCT prediction,
too, was unsupported. Among egalitarian communitarians, science literacy
and numeracy (as reflected in the composite scale Science
literacy/numeracy) showed a small positive correlation with concern
about climate change risks (r=0.08, P=0.03). In contrast, among hierarchical individualists, Science literacy/numeracy is negatively correlated with concern (r=−0.12, P=0.03). Hence, polarization actually becomes larger, not smaller, as science literacy and numeracy increase (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table S4 and Fig. S3).
As the contribution that culture makes to disagreement grows as science
literacy and numeracy increase, it is not plausible to view cultural
cognition as a heuristic substitute for the knowledge or capacities that
SCT views the public as lacking.
Figure 2: SCT prediction versus actual impact of the interaction
between science literacy and numeracy, on the one hand, and cultural
world-views, on the other.
Contrary to SCT’s predictions, highly
science-literate and numerate hierarchical individualists are more
sceptical, not less, of climate change risks. Estimated risk-perception
scores derived from Supplementary Table S4,
Model 3. Hierarchical individualist and egalitarian communitarian
reflect values set, respectively, at +1 s.d. and −1 s.d. on both the
Hierarchy and Individualism cultural-world-view scale predictors. Low
and high reflect values set at −1 and +1 s.d. on the Science
literacy/numeracy scale. Responses on the 0–10 risk scale (M=5.7, s.d.=3.4) were converted to z-scores to promote ease of interpretation. Confidence intervals reflect the 0.95 level of confidence.
To test the generality of this conclusion, we also analysed
subjects’ perceptions of nuclear-power risks. Egalitarian communitarians
and hierarchical individualists were again polarized. Moreover, here,
too, the gap between subjects with these outlooks became larger, not
smaller as scientific literacy and numeracy increased (Supplementary Table S5 and Fig. S3). Extending research that casts doubt on the knowledge-deficit explanation16
for public controversy over climate-change and other environmental
risks, these findings suggest that bounded rationality is an
unsatisfactory explanation as well.
On the contrary, our findings
could be viewed as evidence of how remarkably well-equipped ordinary
individuals are to discern which stances towards scientific information
secure their personal interests. We will elaborate on this
interpretation, which we offer as our own best provisional understanding
of the results of this and related studies, but which we also believe
warrants corroboration by experimental testing. We stress, too, that as
consequential as cultural cognition is for disagreement over climate
change, it does not imply the irrelevance of other, more general
impediments to public engagement with climate-change science, including
trust in communicators and the affective attenuation of risks seen by
many as remote in time and place17.
For
the ordinary individual, the most consequential effect of his beliefs
about climate change is likely to be on his relations with his peers18.
A hierarchical individualist who expresses anxiety about climate change
might well be shunned by his co-workers at an oil refinery in Oklahoma
City. A similar fate will probably befall the egalitarian communitarian
English professor who reveals to colleagues in Boston that she thinks
the scientific consensus on climate change is a hoax. At the same time,
neither the beliefs an ordinary person forms about scientific evidence
nor any actions he takes—as a consumer, say, or democratic voter—will by
itself aggravate or mitigate the dangers of climate change. On his own,
he is just not consequential enough to matter19.
Given how much the ordinary individual depends on peers for
support—material and emotional—and how little impact his beliefs have on
the physical environment, he would probably be best off if he formed
risk perceptions that minimized any danger of estrangement from his
community.
A long-established body of work examining motivated cognition20
supports this conjecture. Both to avoid dissonance and to secure their
group standing, individuals unconsciously seek out and credit
information supportive of “self-defining... values [and] attitudes”21, such as the shared world-views featured in the study of cultural cognition22.
The predictive power of cultural world-views implies that the average
member of the public performs these tasks quite proficiently.
Our data, consistent with that observed in other settings23,
suggest that those with the highest degree of science literacy and
numeracy perform such tasks even more discerningly. Fitting information
to identity-defining commitments makes demands on all manner of
cognition—including both system 1 and system 2 reasoning19,20.
For ordinary citizens, the reward for acquiring greater scientific
knowledge and more reliable technical-reasoning capacities is a greater
facility to discover and use—or explain away—evidence relating to their
groups’ positions.
Even if cultural cognition serves the personal
interests of individuals, this form of reasoning can have a highly
negative impact on collective decision making. What guides individual
risk perception, on this account, is not the truth of those beliefs but
rather their congruence with individuals’ cultural commitments. As a
result, if beliefs about a societal risk such as climate change come to
bear meanings congenial to some cultural outlooks but hostile to others,
individuals motivated to adopt culturally congruent risk perceptions
will fail to converge, or at least fail to converge as rapidly as they
should, on scientific information essential to their common interests in
health and prosperity. Although it is effectively costless for any
individual to form a perception of climate-change risk that is wrong but
culturally congenial, it is very harmful to collective welfare for
individuals in aggregate to form beliefs this way.
One aim of science communication, we submit, should be to dispel this tragedy of the risk-perception commons24.
A communication strategy that focuses only on transmission of sound
scientific information, our results suggest, is unlikely to do that. As
worthwhile as it would be, simply improving the clarity of scientific
information will not dispel public conflict so long as the
climate-change debate continues to feature cultural meanings that divide
citizens of opposing world-views.
It does not follow, however,
that nothing can be done to promote constructive and informed public
deliberations. As citizens understandably tend to conform their beliefs
about societal risk to beliefs that predominate among their peers,
communicators should endeavor to create a deliberative climate in which
accepting the best available science does not threaten any group’s
values. Effective strategies include use of culturally diverse
communicators, whose affinity with different communities enhances their
credibility, and information-framing techniques that invest policy
solutions with resonances congenial to diverse groups22. Perfecting such techniques through a new science of science communication is a public good of singular importance25.
Study subjects consisted of a nationally representative
general population sample of 1,540 US citizens who participated in the
study through the online testing facilities of Knowledge Networks (http://www.knowledgenetworks.com/).
Knowledge Networks is a public opinion research firm with offices
located throughout the US. It maintains an active respondent pool of
some 50,000 adults who are recruited to participate in online surveys
and experiments administered on behalf of academic and governmental
researchers and private businesses. Its recruitment and sampling methods
assure a diverse sample that is demographically representative of the
US population.
We measured respondents’ values using scales associated with studies of the cultural theory of risk4,5.
The first, Hierarchy–Egalitarianism (Hierarchy), consists of
agree–disagree items that indicate attitudes towards social orderings
that connect authority to stratified social roles (for example, ‘We need
to markedly reduce inequalities between the rich and the poor, whites
and people of colour, and men and women’). Items from the second scale,
Individualism–Communitarianism (Individualism), express attitudes
towards social orderings in which the individual is expected to secure
his or her own well-being without assistance or interference from
society versus ones in which society is obliged and empowered to secure
collective welfare in the face of competing individual interests (for
example, ‘Government should put limits on the choices individuals can
make so they do not get in the way of what is good for society’).
We measured respondents’ science literacy with National Science Foundation’s (NSF) Science and Engineering Indicators26.
Focused on physics and biology (for example, ‘Electrons are smaller
than atoms [true/false]’; ‘Antibiotics kill viruses as well as bacteria
[true/false]’), the NSF Indicators are widely used as an index of public
comprehension of basic science27.
We
measured subjects’ numeracy—their capacity to comprehend and use
quantitative information—with fourteen mathematical word problems11,28, 29 (for example, ‘A bat and a ball cost $1.10 in total. The bat costs $1.00
more than the ball. How much does the ball cost?’). We combined
responses to the NSF Indicators and the numeracy questions into a
composite scale (α=0.85), labelled
Science literacy/numeracy, to avoid collinearity in multivariate
analyses of their association with respondents’ risk perceptions30.
Those
risk perceptions were measured with items labelled GWRISK and NUKERISK,
which asked respondents to indicate ‘How much risk’ they believed
‘climate change’ and ‘nuclear power,’ respectively, ‘pose[] to human
health, safety or prosperity’ on a 0 (no risk) to 10 (extreme risk)
scale. Risk-perception items that conform to this format are known to
elicit responses that correlate highly with ones targeted at more
specific factual beliefs about the hazards of putative risk sources and
are thus routinely used as a parsimonious focus for analysis of variance
in risk perceptions8, 9.
Study hypotheses were tested by ordinary least-squares linear regression (SupplementaryTables S4 and S5).
Predictors included the cultural-world-view scales, Science
literacy/numeracy and appropriate cross-product interaction terms. To
promote visual comprehension of the variance associated with various
predictors, responses to GWRISK (M=5.7, s.d.=3.4) and NUKERISK (M=6.1, s.d.=3.0) were transformed into z-scores.
Full item wording for all measures and the multivariate regression outputs are reported in the Supplementary Information.
Yale University, Yale Law School, PO Box 208215, New Haven, Connecticut 06520, USA
Dan M. Kahan
The Ohio State University, 235 Psychology Building, 1835 Neil Avenue, Columbus, Ohio 43210, USA
Ellen Peters
Cultural Cognition Project Lab, Yale University, Yale Law School, PO Box 208215, New Haven, Connecticut, USA
Maggie Wittlin &
Lisa Larrimore Ouellette
Decision Research, 1201 Oak Street, Suite 200, Eugene, Oregon 97401, USA
Paul Slovic
George Washington University, 2000 H Street, N.W, Washington DC 20052, USA
Donald Braman
Temple University, 1719 North Broad St., Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19122, USA
Gregory Mandel
Contributions:
D.M.K., E.P., M.W. and L.L.O. contributed to all aspects of the
paper, including study design, statistical analysis and writing and
revisions. P.S., D.B. and G.M. contributed to the design of the study,
to substantive analysis of the results and to revisions of the paper.
Competing financial interests:
The authors declare no competing financial interests. http://www.nature.com/nclimate/journal/vaop/ncurrent/full/nclimate1547.html
I know you’re fed up
Like a lead up for us
All they talk about is
What is going down?
What’s been messed up for us?
When I look around I see blue skies
I see butterflies for us
Listen to the sound and lose it
Its sweet music and dance with me
There is beauty in the world
So much beauty in the world
Always beauty in the world
So much beauty in the world
Shake your booty boys and girls for the beauty in the world
Pick your diamond pick your pearl there is beauty in the world
All together now
We need more lovin’
We need more money, they say
Change is gonna come
Like the weather
They say forever
They say
When they’re in between
Notice the blue skies
Notice the butterflies
Notice me
Stop and smell the flowers
And lose it the sweet music and dance with me
There is beauty in the world
So much beauty in the world
Always beauty in the world
There is beauty in the world
Shake your booty boys and girls for the beauty in the world
Pick your diamond pick your pearl there is beauty in the world
All together now
Heya throw your hands up and holla
Throw your hands up and holla
When you don’t know what to do
Don’t know if you’ll make it through
Remember god is giving you beauty in the world
So love (Beauty in the world)
Yeah love (Beauty in the world)
There is beauty in the world (Beauty in the world)
Beauty in the world (Beauty in the world)
Shake your booty boys and girls (Boys and Girls)
All the beauty in the world (Beauty in the world)
Pick your diamond pick your pearl (Pick your pearl)
There is beauty in the world (Beauty in the world)
All together now
Yeah love
Yeah love
Oh love
All together now
Hey baby when I’m looking at you
I know it’s fact is true
There is hope for love
There is beauty in the world
Hey baby
Hey baby when I’m looking at you
I know this vibe is true
There’s love
There’s hope for love
There’s beauty in the world
No comments:
Post a Comment